Group process gains and losses
Nunamaker et. al. [1991]
distinguish five factors that lead to group process gains and fifteen sources
of group process losses.
Group process gains
- More information. A group as a whole has more information than any one
member.
- Synergy. A group member uses information in a way that the original holder did not
because that member has different information or skills.
- More objective evaluation. Groups are better at catching errors than are the
individuals who proposed ideas.
- Simulation. Working as part of a group may stimulate and encourage individuals to
perform better.
- Learning. Group members may learn from and imitate more skilled members to
improve performance.
- Air time fragmentation. The group must partition available speaking time among
members.
- Attenuation blocking. Occurs when group members who are prevented from
contributing comments as they are occur, forget or suppress them later in the meeting,
because they seem less original, relevant, or important.
- Concentration blocking. Fewer comments are made because group members
concentrate on remembering comments (rather than thinking of new ones) until they can
contribute them.
- Attention blocking. New comments are not generated because group members must
constantly listen to others speak and cannot pause to think.
- Failure to remember. Group members lack focus on communication, missing or
forgetting the contributions of others.
- Conformance pressure. Group members are reluctant to criticise the comments of
others due to politeness or fear of reprisals.
- Evaluation Apprehension. Fear of negative evaluation causes group members to
withhold ideas and comments.
- Free riding. A group member notices that his colleagues contribute more than
satisfactory and therefore decides not to participate.
Often associated with the free rider problem, is the assurance dilemma.
A group member may have problems regarding the contributions of the others:
do they take on their fair share? When this is not guaranteed, he may decide not to
participate any further. Particularly in large groups, these effects may occur yet remain
unnoticed.
- Cognitive inertia. Discussion moves along one train of thought without deviating
because group members refrain from contributing comments that are not directly related
to the current discussion.
- Socialising. Nontask discussion reduces task performance, although some socialising
is usually necessary for effective functioning.
- Domination. Some group member(s) exercise undue influence or monopolise the
group's time in an unproductive manner.
- Information overload. Information is presented faster than it can be processed.
- Coordination problems. Difficulty integrating group members' contributions because
the group does not have an appropriate strategy, which can lead to disfunctional cycling
or incomplete discussions resulting in premature decisions.
- Incomplete use of information. Incomplete access to and use of information
necessary for successful task completion.
- Incomplete task analysis. Incomplete analysis and understanding of the task resulting
in superficial discussions.
The mentioned forms of blocking, attenuation blocking, concentration blocking, and
attention blocking, are subelements of production blocking.
Group decisions
Besides factors that occur on an individual level, Meijer [1994]
identifies three processes which influence all group members at the same time.
Deficiencies in group decision making appear as a consequence of a process called
groupthink, a distorted manner of thinking that makes it hard for group members
to make rational decisions. Groupthink is likely to occur in unanimous, cohesive groups.
Other antecedent conditions for groupthink include structural faults of the organisation
(e.g., the group is insulated or all members share the same social background and
ideology), as well as a provocative situational context.
Symptoms of groupthink include:
- Overestimation of the group, including an illusion of invulnerability as well as
a firm belief in the inherent morality of the group.
- Narrow mindedness, which manifests itself in collective rationalisations and
stereotypes of out-groups.
- Pressures toward uniformity. This finds expression in self-censorship, an illusion
of unanimity, and direct pressure on dissenters.
Apparently, groups make more extreme decisions than individuals. They express either
very risky or extremely risk-averse behaviour. This phenomenon is called group
polarisation. The group polarisation effect is illustrated in the following figure.
When the pregroup attitude of the individuals that are to form a group inclines
toward risk-seeking, the postgroup reaction will express more extreme risk-seeking as
a consequence of the risky shift. On the other hand, when the group members'
pregroup attitude reflects risk-aversion, the postgroup reaction will be extremely risk-
aversive due to the cautious shift.
The third process is de-individuation, which is defined by
Lea and Spears [1991] as:
the process whereby submergence in a group produces anonymity
and a loss of identity, and a consequent weakening of social norms and constraints.
Generally, de-individuation rears its head when the group is large and when its members
are excited. De-individuation is not always a negative process. Meijer [1994]
mentions people in a stadium or in a theatre; apparently, in a crowd they can feel quite comfortable
in a state of de-individuation.
However, de-individuation is undesirable in a working situation. Therefore groups engaged in
cooperative work should be kept small in order to identify the actions undertaken by individual
members. Moreover, all group members should be treated as unique individuals and be aware of
their own responsibilities.
Sjoerd Michels, Tilburg, The Netherlands